Skip to main content
Admissions Toolkits
Look into Laurier

Join us at Laurier

Being a Golden Hawk means more than just cheering on our (really good) varsity teams – it means being a student who cares about your community, who works hard in the classroom, and who takes advantage of all the learning opportunities that can happen outside the classroom, too.


We’ve all been there — at the theatre for an evening with friends who unapologetically veto your movie suggestion.

Woefully, and despite your best effort to convince the group otherwise, you ended up sitting bemused through what felt like an eternity of moving images.

Did the concession to give up your movie pick influence your clout within the group? How did you feel about yourself and your friends?

According to Laurier Associate Professor of Psychology Noam Miller, you may have a thing or two to learn from fish. Collective animal cognition and personality is Miller’s expertise, and with funding from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, he is exploring how groups can cause missed learning opportunities, future decisions and collective behaviour.

“For many species, including humans, membership in groups is a very important factor in every day life,” says Miller. “Your cognition, everything that your brain does — from perception and learning to making decisions — is altered or affected when it happens in a group.”

In his collective cognition lab in Laurier’s Department of Psychology, Miller works with zebrafish, a small schooling species from northern India and Nepal used by geneticists, developmental biologists and neuroscientists.

Following his work in animal behaviour during his undergraduate studies, Miller worked with individual zebrafish during his doctorate at the University of Toronto. Finding it frustrating to train them, Miller decided to study something they do on their own — schooling and swimming in groups. Examining how groups of zebrafish move around together and how their group behaviour depends on environmental factors became central to his research.

Researchers can observe and measure several different dimensions of animal behaviour, including aggression, boldness or explorativeness, and sociability. And similar to humans who exhibit behavioural syndromes, researchers can find consistent patterns and differences between individual fish.

“We can classify and quantify their group interactions without complicated scenarios,” says Miller. “If we take a group of fish and characterize them on their various behaviours, we can ask, ‘What will happen if we group all of the shy ones together? What if we build a group that’s really bold? And what if we build a group that’s split between bold and shy fish?’ These are all interesting questions we’ve been exploring.”

Miller is exploring whether there is an evolutionary advantage to having a certain characteristic such as boldness or shyness vary within a population. Different phenotypes may be advantageous in certain environments, such as making a group faster or more accurate at foraging or avoiding predators.

Miller is also interested in how and why fish naturally choose their own groups.

“We don’t pick our children’s friends and say, ‘Okay, you’re bold so I’m going to put you in a group with other bold kids.’ How that’s decided is really complicated, but generally children go into the school population and end up with a group of friends,” says Miller.

He is interested in simulating this experience in fish — letting them choose their own group and then asking what the characteristics of the individuals of these groups look like.

“Human cases are a lot more complicated than animal situations, but at least fundamentally we hope that there are similar processes happening,” says Miller.

At a fundamental level, he says the kinds of problems that groups need to solve are often identical. Both human and animal groups have to stay together and not disperse, and they have to avoid bunching on top of each other, which raises issues over group movement and the need for “traffic rules.”

In addition to movement, there are important dynamics of cooperation and competition common to both human and animal groups.

“If I’m following a group to get food, will there actually be any left when I get there? Maybe I shouldn’t be following; maybe I should be leading,” says Miller. This leads to the question of how conformity and collective influence impact the individual decisions of humans and animals: they must constantly balance the individual information they have with the social information they have — or what everyone else is doing.

How cognition is shaped by its social context — and how different animals solve the same challenge of balancing personal preference versus the cohesion of the group — is a key research question being studied by Miller and his lab.

It’s something worth considering the next time you’re stuck watching your least preferred movie pick.

About the photo

The photo used in this story is by Ariana Strandburg-Peshkin. It shows zebrafish, each three to four centimetres long, tagged using a fluorescent elastomer. Using UV light to make the tags fluoresce, Miller’s lab can identify individual fish and their behaviour in tanks.


×

We see you are accessing our website on IE8. We recommend you view in Chrome, Safari, Firefox or IE9+ instead.

×